A
few days ago I was debating on a Facebook group about the rise of
intolerance to those who are not white or appear different. One
comment stands out. Someone I know and have quite a lot of time for
made this point. He said “You should be proud of your race Martyn”
Since then I have begun to reflect on the myth of race . I have seen
over the years how our language is racially loaded and subject to
cultural prejudice. People often say “I dont see any evidence of
worsening hate crimes “ its because they dont experience because
they are not the minorities that they experience it and they fail to
understand that as their culture and outlook is seen as normal and
everyday they do not see it. I am white, middle class and
heterosexual so it took me the best part of 30years to understand and
recognise how prejudice and discrimination works. We haver to think,
reflect , gain information and talk to others before we understand.
This
morning I went as I always do to pick up my paper at Neath Railway
Station. I was chatting to a friend about the Tory leadership
contest. Suddenly I head a voice saying “Whoever wins there still
here those Muslims ..they haven't gone back yet” I had images of
deportation, ethnic cleansing, the deep hatred and loathing that has
been released since June 23. Ordinary people have been confused ,
manipulated, made afraid. I thought back in history to the massive
deportation of Germans, Poles , Ukrainians, of Greek and Turkish
populations kicked out of Turkey and Greece at the end of World War
1.
Their
is a myth of race that confuses many people. There is a confusion
between ethnicity and culture, genetics and belief. It is
fundamentally depressing and we live in a sea of ignorance and
prejudice.
I
have a genetic mix that is Irish, Welsh, Jewish, there are Greek and
Italian traces. There are also Scandinavian and East European. So
where would you send me you bigots?
I
have found some old notes I made or discovered because of my interest
in Social Anthropology and Cultural Studies. I hope these things have
some effect. How long will it me before we see the little signs
appearing in Neath that Say “No Irish, no Blacks , no Dogs” I
wonder......... a sobering thought on a Friday morning.
How
difficult is it to jettison the idea of race as biology?
To
understand why the idea of race is a biological myth requires a major
paradigm shift - an absolutely paradigm shift, a shift in
perspective. And for me, it's like seeing what it must have been like
to understand that the world isn't flat. The world looks flat to our
eyes. And perhaps I can invite you to a mountaintop or to a plain,
and you can look out the window at the horizon, and see, "Oh,
what I thought was flat I can see a curve in now." And that race
is not based on biology, but race is rather an idea that we ascribe
to biology.
That's
quite shocking to a lot of individuals. When you look and you think
you see race, to be told that no, you don't see race, you just think
you see race, you know, it's based on your cultural lens - that's
extremely challenging.
What's
heartening is that so many students love it. They feel liberated by
beginning to understand that, in fact, whiteness is a cultural
construction, that race is a cultural construction, that we really
are fundamentally alike. It's our politics, it's political economy,
it's an old ideology that tends to separate us out. It's institutions
that have been born with the idea of race and racism that tend to
separate us out.
Young
children today, kids today, in my experience, love it that we can
have some common humanity, that we can come together as one, that
this idea of biological race is a myth that's separating us. They
love the idea that there's really some wall that can be smashed down
and help bring us together.
What's
wrong with classifying by race as biology?
Scientists
have actually been saying for quite a while that race, as biology,
doesn't exist - that there's no biological basis for race. And that
is in the facts of biology, the facts of non-concordance, the facts
of continuous variation, the recentness of our evolution, the way
that we all commingle and come together, how genes flow, and perhaps
especially in the fact that most variation occurs within race versus
between races or among races, suggesting that there's no
generalizability to race. There is no centre there; there is no there
there in the centre. It's fluid.
But
many individuals will say, "Well, that's okay, at least it's an
approximation. It at least gives us a way to classify. Hey, you know,
our head size may be continuous and shoe size may be continuous, but
we developed a way to classify people by hat size and shoe size. And
it kind of works. Your shoe may be a little bit crunchy but you
basically know to go in and start somewhere, So what's wrong with
doing it for race?"
And
I'll tell you, there's a couple things that are wrong with it, where
that analogy really breaks down. We've developed a universal system
for thinking about hat size that's measurable, for example. So you
can go into Sao Paulo Brazil and the hat merchants there have the
same scale that the hat merchants do in Cambridge, Massachusetts. And
we can have universality because it's objective, it's measurable,
we're just measuring the circumference around the head. It doesn't
change culturally from one place to another.
But
think about race and its universality or lack thereof. Where is your
measurement device? There is no way to measure race first. We
sometimes do it by skin color. Other people may do it by hair
texture. Other people may have the dividing lines different in terms
of skin color. What's black in the United States is not what's black
in Brazil or what's black in South Africa. What was black in 1940 is
different from what is black in 2000. Certainly, with the evolution
of whiteness, what was white in 1920 - as a Jew I was not white then,
but I'm white now, so white has changed tremendously.
There's
no stability and constancy. That's life. That's fine as social ideas
go, that we all have our individual classification systems and may
use them, but for science, it's death. It does not work. Science is
based on generalizability, it's based on consistency, it's based on
reproducibility. If you have none of that, you have junk science.
What
is non-concordance and what does that tell us about race?
For
race to have meaning, for race to be more than skin-deep, for race to
be more than a typology, one has to have concordance. In other words,
skin color needs to reflect things that are deeper in the body, under
the skin. But, in fact, human variation is rather non-concordant.
I'll
give you an example of concordance. Height is actually quite
concordant with weight. As we get taller, we gain weight, we have
more weight. One aspect of size is concordant with another aspect of
size.
But
most of human variation is non-concordant. Skin color or eye color or
hair color is not correlated with height or weight. And they're
definitely not correlated with more complex traits like intelligence
or athletic performance. Those things evolve and develop in entirely
different ways. Just as skin color develops in a different way from
size, intelligence, athletic performance, other traits develop in
different and independent ways.
A
map of skin color gradients looks sort of like the map of
temperature. It gets lighter, as you go towards the poles and it's
darker near the equator. But then take a map of, say, the
distribution of blood type A. Looks entirely different. There's no
relationship between the two maps. The distributions are
non-concordant. Simply, one is not related to the other.
When
we adopt a racial view, we have to see concordance. And perhaps if we
don't see it, we make it up. Because if there's no concordance, there
is no race. So, racist scientists, for example, have to see a
concordance between skin color and IQ, otherwise there's no meaning
there, there is no there there. There's nothing under the skin. Race
stops at the color of your skin.
What's
at risk? Quite a bit is at risk. It's how we see each other, how we
respect each other. It's about understanding that somebody from one
town in Poland, and somebody from the next town in Poland, could be
more different from each other than a Pole and a person from South
Africa.
It's
about knowing that our assumptions about difference and who we're
related to and who we're most alike may be entirely wrong. It's
ultimately about a revolution in how we think about human difference
and similarity.
How
much human variation falls within any population, and how much
between "races"?
Richard
Lewontin did an amazing piece of work which he published in 1972, in
a famous article called "The Apportionment of Human Variation."
Literally what he tried to do was see how much genetic variation
showed up at three different levels.
One
level was the variation that showed up among or between purported
races. And the conventional idea is that quite a bit of variation
would show up at that level. And then he also explored two other
levels at the same time. How much variation occurred within a race,
but between or among sub-groups within that purported race.
So,
for instance, in Europe, how much variation would there be between
the Germans, the Finns and the Spanish? Or how much variation could
we call local variation, occurring within an ethnicity such as the
Navaho or Hopi or the Chatua.
And
the amazing result was that, on average, about 85% of the variation
occurred within any given group. The vast majority of that variation
was found at a local level. In fact, groups like the Finns are not
homogeneous - they actually contain, I guess one could literally say,
85% of the genetic diversity of the world.
Secondly,
of that remaining 15%, about half of that, seven and a half percent
or so, was found to be still within the continent, but just between
local populations; between the Germans and the Finns and the Spanish.
So, now we're over 90%, something like 93% of variation actually
occurs within any given continental group. And only about 6-7% of
that variation occurs between "races," leaving one to say
that race actually explains very little of human variation.
You
know, geography perhaps is the better way to explain that 15% more
than race or anything else. For instance, there can be accumulations
of genes in one place in the globe, and not another.
But,
for the most part, you know that basic human plan is really a basic
human plan, and is found almost anywhere in the world. Most variation
is found locally within any group. Why don't we believe that? Because
we happen to ascribe great significance to skin color, and a few
other physical cues that tell us that that's not so. And, in fact
though, these may happen to be a few of the things that do widely
vary from place to place. But, that's not true under the skin.
Rather, quite another story is told by looking at genes under the
skin.
Are
there boundaries dividing populations?
The
idea of race, of course, assumes that there are set boundaries
between the races, but we know that to be untrue. You know, there's
no racial boundary that's ever been found. Any trait that one looks
at, one tends to see gradual variation from one group to another. The
facts of human variation are that it's continuous, it's not lumped
into three or four or five racial groups.
One
of the ways to begin to see a different paradigm, to see that the
world really isn't divided into three or four or five types of
individuals, is to really try to locate those individuals, to find
them and to locate the racial boundaries between them. You could take
any characteristic you want, but the most frequently used is skin
color. We think that each type of person has a different skin color.
But
do this as a thought experiment: start off in northern Scandinavian,
say northern Finland, and take a walk in your mind through
Scandinavia, perhaps into Germany, down through Germany into southern
Europe, through the Mediterranean perhaps, circle around until you
get to Algeria, into northern Africa, and continue on your way down
towards the equator, and finally from the equator to South Africa.
The
challenge would be to say where does one race begin, and where does
another race end. Or even where does dark skin begin, and light skin
end? Or, perhaps as you leave the equator, where does light skin
begin to show up again? In fact, what you find is a rather subtle
gradation in skin colors. This is called "clinal variation",
and it's really quite like what you see in your weather maps of
temperature in the back of the Sun, or your 11 o'clock weather
forecast, where you can see how temperature grades change ever so
slightly as you go from north to south. Well, skin color is actually
quite the same thing. It varies clinally - continuously. There is no
abrupt change from one skin color to the next.
How
is human genetic difference - and similarity - traced to our history?
We
basically are the same plan, and we don't need to alter our plan. In
fact, one of the hallmarks of humans is that we're flexible. We are
built with this very flexible brain and flexible structure that lets
us go into a lot of new situations without needing to genetically
adapt to it. We're kind of like the Swiss Army knife of species. We
can apply culture and our ideas to conquer different environments.
When we go into the cold we don't need to grow hair. We just need to
find a buffalo skin to put on. Or better yet, we invent central
heating.
As
best we know, humans started in Africa. And they had a lot of time
working out what they were going to be like in Africa. And through
that time of working out what they were going to be like in Africa,
they began to diverge and change slowly, ever so slowly.
Some
of that change may have been due to selective pressures in different
parts of Africa. And Africa is a very diverse place, with different
climates, different eco-zones. There may have been some selection
from that, and selection from diseases, with sickle cell being one
concrete example of that, since sickle cell is a response to malaria.
And malaria is not something that's a big problem throughout Africa,
but is a huge problem, a huge selective force, in certain parts of
Africa.
Adapting
to different environments and circumstances is one way that we see
change develop. However, it's probably not the major thing that makes
us different clinally, geographically different. After all, we are a
young species, and we're generalists.
Another
way we change is more or less by random flow of genes. This is one of
the big hallmarks of humans, that we tend to be very mobile. We've
always been very mobile. And our genes are even more mobile. We may
not move, but our genes may move because somebody we mated with, or
the grandchild of somebody we've mated with, that person moves. And
that person's great, great, great grandchild moves, and so our genes
are constantly on the move and literally moving around the planet.
That
was the story 100,000 years ago. It was the story 75,000 years ago.
It's the story 50,000 years ago, and up to the present. We've had
maybe 100,000 years of having genes move out and mix and re-assort in
countless different ways.
Some
of those movements may follow major migrations as agricultural people
came into Europe, as people crossed the Bering Strait and came into
the Americas. But, other movements are much more subtle. They're
smaller groups of individuals that moved, or their genes moved from
place to place, and time to time. We're constantly out-migrating and
mating outside our group, responding to the urge to merge. And that
happens all the time. And that is us. So, what you end up with,
mathematically and in reality, are subtle gradations; one gene
grading one way, another gene grading another way.
How
does Social Darwinism - and race - rationalize inequality?
Social
Darwinism was really just an explanation for the order of things. We
had to come up with an explanation for why certain Europeans had more
access to power and were wealthier than others.
So
we use nature as an explanation for what we saw, or seem to think we
saw in nature: those who were more aggressive, or more intelligent,
got things, and those who weren't got less. So that became the
continuing justification for taking over lands, for slavery, for
competition. That competition was good. And to the winners went the
spoils. And there's no need to feel guilt or anxiety about that,
because that's natural, it's a reflection of nature. And to the
winner go the advantages of having been a winner.
I
think there are many, many legacies of social Darwinism today. We
don't see how uneven the playing field is, for one. We don't
acknowledge that individuals grow up with less advantage and more
advantage. We seem to think that i we all are born with a blank slate
and an equal ability to get ahead.
What
about studies equating race with intelligence?
Scientific
work abetting the idea that race is real, typological, and
hierarchically arranged is actually rather an old occupation, you
know. In the mid-1840s we see Samuel Morton measuring crania to get
at cranial capacity and then to try to rank the races on the amount
of cranial capacity they have, and to equate that with racial
differences and intelligence.
And
really about every 20 years somebody else comes along, almost with a
best-seller, perhaps with a new method for measuring intelligence,
ultimately to show that there is a ranking in intelligence, usually
with whites up top.
The
most recent effort was The Bell Curve, which came out in 1994 and
literally reached number two on the best-seller list in 1994, behind
a book, by the way, written by Pope John Paul. The Bell Curve threw a
couple spins into this. One is that it actually promoted Asians as
being closer to the top, also broke down whites a little bit more.
But fundamentally it was the same type of book as was written by
Morton in 1850; you use the same basic methods and the same basic
logic.
Their
argument went something like this: there is something called
intelligence that we can put our fingers on, that we can measure;
intelligence is some sort of univariable; it's one-dimensional. That
intelligence then is measurable by something called an intelligence
test that actually measures intelligence. And then that intelligence
is highly heritable; it's something we really do get in our
chromosomes, in our genes; it comes to us that way, it is highly
heritable. Then one has to say that there is such a thing as white,
black, and Asian, or whatever groups you're comparing, that they are
real, that they are measurable, that they are reproducible.
But
then let's to back and look at the assumptions again. Is there a
white group, a black group, an Asian group? Are these reproducible?
Are they trained equally? Can we really measure a variable called
intelligence? Is it really something that's not affected by
environment, about how we're trained, how we grow up, what
stimulation we have by children?
I'll
give you an example. One test has shown that just a little bit of
lead in the blood can affect intelligence - a little bit of lead in
the blood, prenatal, can affect intelligence by easily eight points
on an intelligence score. Are we to believe that those factors were
unimportant in looking at the differences in IQ scores? Of course
not.
The
assumptions that go into believing that there are racial differences
in intelligence are absurd ones. They're ones that we shouldn't even
be coming close to as scientists. The chief one is that here's such a
thing as race, that there are races, and that a score on a test, an
average group score, has any meaning for an individual.
Why
is it important to overturn the idea of race as biology?
We
live in racial smog. This is a world of racial smog. We can't help
but breathe that smog. Everybody breathes it. But what's nice is that
you can recognize that you are breathing that smog, and that's the
first step.
We
all live in a racialised society. And individuals of color are
exposed to it more obviously, with more virulence, more force, than
anybody is.
But
what is important is that race is a very salient social and
historical concept, a social and historical idea. It's shaped
institutions, it's shaped our legal system, it shapes interactions in
law offices and housing offices and in medical schools, in dentist's
offices. It shapes that. And I think by stripping the biology from
it, by stripping the idea that race is somehow based in biology, we
show the emperor to have no clothes, we show race for what it is:
it's an idea that's constantly being reinvented, and it's up to us
about how we want to invent it and go ahead and reinvent it. But it's
up to us to do it.
Racism
rests in part on the idea that race is biology; it is based on
biology. So, the biology becomes an excuse for social differences.
The social differences become naturalized in biology. It's not that
our institutions cause differences in mortality; it's that there
really are biological differences between the races.
So,
until we address that there is no race in biology, that race is an
idea that we ascribe to biology, that there's no race there, there's
a possibility that well-meaning and not-so-well meaning individuals
will drag that up and will inevitably put that in our faces as the
reasons why there are differences in life circumstances between
different groups.
I consider myself a human, no other categorisation necessary.
ReplyDeleteI consider myself a human, no other categorisation is necessary.
ReplyDelete